Languages and PsychotherapyThe Effect of Foreign Language on Fear Extinction

  1. Isabel Ortigosa-Beltrán
  2. Jon Andoni Duñabeitia
  3. Víctor Costumero
  4. Diana Castilla
  5. Irene Jaén
  6. Albert Costa
  7. Azucena García-Palacios
Revista:
Psicothema

ISSN: 0214-9915 1886-144X

Año de publicación: 2023

Volumen: 35

Número: 1

Páginas: 30-40

Tipo: Artículo

DOI: 10.7334/PSICOTHEMA2022.326 DIALNET GOOGLE SCHOLAR lock_openAcceso abierto editor

Otras publicaciones en: Psicothema

Resumen

Usar un idioma extranjero puede influir la modulación de las emociones, pero no está claro si su uso podría influir diferentes procesos psicoterapéuticos. Este estudio explora el efecto de la lengua extranjera en la extinción del miedo. Método: Durante el condicionamiento, parte de los estímulos presentados se asociaron a amenaza, mientras los participantes realizaban una tarea de cuenta regresiva en un contexto de lengua nativa. En la fase de extinción, se realizó la misma tarea en su lengua nativa/extranjera, y se informó de que ya no habría amenaza. Se recogieron autoinformes de miedo, así como dilatación de pupila y actividad electrodérmica como medidas fisiológicas de excitación. Resultados: La extinción fue satisfactoria, como muestran una mayor dilatación pupilar y los autoinformes de miedo al comparar las condiciones de amenaza y neutral en la fase de condicionamiento, y no encontrar diferencias significativas durante la extinción. Aunque el grupo de lengua extranjera presentó una mayor excitación, la extinción del miedo ocurre independientemente del contexto lingüístico. Conclusiones: La extinción del miedo mediante instrucciones verbales es igualmente eficaz en contexto de lengua extranjera y nativa. Estos resultados invitan a seguir reuniendo pruebas sobre el papel de las lenguas extranjeras mediante paradigmas con aplicaciones clínicas.

Referencias bibliográficas

  • Alnæs, D., Sneve, M. H., Espeseth, T., Endestad, T., van de Pavert, S. H. P., & Laeng, B. (2014). Pupil size signals mental effort deployed during multiple object tracking and predicts brain activity in the dorsal attention network and the locus coeruleus. Journal of Vision, 14(4), 1-20. https://doi.org/10.1167/14.4.1
  • Anooshian, L. J., & Hertel, P. T. (1994). Emotionality in free recall: Language specificity in bilingual memory. Cognition & Emotion, 8(6), 503-514. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699939408408956
  • Aragno, A., & Schlachet, P. J. (1996). Accessibility of early experience through the language of origin: A theoretical integration. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 13(1), 23-34. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0079636
  • Atlas, L. Y. (2019). How instructions shape aversive learning: higher order knowledge, reversal learning, and the role of the amygdala. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 26, 121-129. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.12.008
  • Buxbaum, E. (1949). The role of a second language in the formation of ego and superego. The Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 18(3), 279-289.
  • Bradley, M. M., Miccoli, L., Escrig, M. A., & Lang, P. J. (2008). The pupil as a measure of emotional arousal and autonomic activation. Psychophysiology, 45(4), 602-607. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00654.x
  • Brouwer, S. (2021). The interplay between emotion and modality in the ForeignLanguage effect on moral decision making. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 24(2), 223-230. https://doi.org/10.1017/S136672892000022X
  • Caldwell-Harris, C. L. (2015). Emotionality differences between a native and foreign language: Implications for everyday life. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24(3), 214-219. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721414566268
  • Caldwell-Harris, C. L., & Ayçiçeği-Dinn, A. (2009). Emotion and lying in a non-native language. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 71(3), 193-204. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.09.006
  • Čavar, F., & Tytus, A. E. (2018). Moral judgement and foreign language effect: when the foreign language becomes the second language. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 39(1), 17-28. https://doi.org/10.1080/01434632.2017.1304397
  • Corey, J. D., Hayakawa, S., Foucart, A., Aparici, M., Botella, J., Costa, A., & Keysar, B. (2017). Our moral choices are foreign to us. Journal of experimental psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 43(7), Article 1109. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000356.
  • Costa, A., Foucart, A., Hayakawa, S., Aparici, M., Apesteguia, J., Heafner, J., & Keysar, B. (2014). Your morals depend on language. PloS one, 9(4), Article e94842. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0094842
  • Costa, A., Vives, M. L., & Corey, J. D. (2017). On language processing shaping decision making. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26(2), 146- 151. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721416680263
  • Craske, M. G., Hermans, D., & Vervliet, B. (2018). State-of-the-art and future directions for extinction as a translational model for fear and anxiety. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1742), Article 20170025. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2017.0025
  • de Bruin, A., Samuel, A.G., & Duñabeitia, J.A. (2018). Voluntary language switching: When and why do bilinguals switch between their languages? Journal of Memory and Language, 103, 28-43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2018.07.005
  • de Bruin, A., Samuel, A.G., & Duñabeitia, J.A. (2020). Examining bilingual language switching across the lifespan in cued and voluntary switching contexts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 46(8), 759-788. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000746
  • Driver, M. Y. (2020). Switching codes and shifting morals: How code-switching and emotion affect moral judgment. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 25(3), 905-921. https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2020.1730763
  • Duñabeitia, J. A., & Costa, A. (2015). Lying in a native and foreign language. Psychonomic bulletin & review, 22(4), 1124-1129. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0781-4
  • Dylman, A. S., & Champoux-Larsson, M. F. (2020). It’s (not) all Greek to me: Boundaries of the foreign language effect. Cognition, 196, Article 104148. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2019.104148
  • Eilola, T. M., & Havelka, J. (2011). Behavioural and physiological responses to the emotional and taboo Stroop tasks in native and non-native speakers of English. International Journal of Bilingualism, 15(3), 353-369. https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006910379263
  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A. G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G* Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior research methods, 41(4), 1149-1160. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149
  • Fernández-López, M., & Perea, M. (2020). Language does not modulate fake news credibility, but emotion does. Psicológica, 41(2), 84-102. https://doi.org/10.2478/psicolj-2020-0005
  • García-Palacios, A., Costa, A., Castilla, D., del Río, E., Casaponsa, A., & Duñabeitia, J. A. (2018). The effect of foreign language in fear acquisition. Scientific reports, 8(1), 1-8. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-19352-8
  • Geipel, J., Hadjichristidis, C., & Surian, L. (2016). Foreign language affects the contribution of intentions and outcomes to moral judgment. Cognition, 154, 34-39. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.05.010
  • Grégoire, L., & Greening, S. G. (2020). Fear of the known: Semantic generalisation of fear conditioning across languages in bilinguals. Cognition and Emotion, 34(2), 352–358. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2019.1604319
  • Gyurak, A., Goodkind, M. S., Madan, A., Kramer, J. H., Miller, B. L., & Levenson, R. W. (2009). Do tests of executive functioning predict ability to downregulate emotions spontaneously and when instructed to suppress?. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 9(2), 144-152. https://doi.org/10.3758/CABN.9.2.144.
  • Harris, C. L., Ayçíçeğ, A., & Gleason, J. B. (2003). Taboo words and reprimands elicit greater autonomic reactivity in a first language than in a second language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24(4), 561-579. https://doi.org/10.1017.S0142716403000286
  • Hess, E. H., & Polt, J. M. (1960). Pupil size as related to interest value of visual stimuli. Science, 132(3423), 349-350. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.132.3423.349
  • Iacozza, S., Costa, A., & Duñabeitia, J. A. (2017). What do your eyes reveal about your foreign language? Reading emotional sentences in a native and foreign language. PloS one, 12(10), e0186027. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0186027
  • Keysar, B., Hayakawa, S. L., & An, S. G. (2012). The foreign-language effect: Thinking in a foreign tongue reduces decision biases. Psychological science, 23(6), 661-668. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611432178
  • Kreibig, S. D. (2010). Autonomic nervous system activity in emotion: A review. Biological psychology, 84(3), 394-421. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.03.010
  • Lonsdorf, T. B., Menz, M. M., Andreatta, M., Fullana, M. A., Golkar, A., Haaker, J., ... & Drexler, S. M. (2017). Don’t fear ‘fear conditioning’: Methodological considerations for the design and analysis of studies on human fear acquisition, extinction, and return of fear. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 77, 247-285. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.02.026
  • Luck, C. C., & Lipp, O. V. (2015). To remove or not to remove? Removal of the unconditional stimulus electrode does not mediate instructed extinction effects. Psychophysiology, 52(9), 1248-1256. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12452
  • Luck, C. C., & Lipp, O. V. (2016). Instructed extinction in human fear conditioning: History, recent developments, and future directions. Australian Journal of Psychology, 68(3), 209-227. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajpy.12135
  • Marian, V., Blumenfeld, H. K., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2007). The Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP-Q): Assessing language profiles in bilinguals and multilinguals. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50(4), 940-967. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2007/067)
  • Mechias, M. L., Etkin, A., & Kalisch, R. (2010). A meta-analysis of instructed fear studies: implications for conscious appraisal of threat. Neuroimage, 49(2), 1760-1768. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.09.040
  • Mertens, G., Boddez, Y., Sevenster, D., Engelhard, I. M., & De Houwer, J. (2018). A review on the effects of verbal instructions in human fear conditioning: Empirical findings, theoretical considerations, and future directions. Biological Psychology, 137, 49-64. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018.07.002
  • Morawetz, C., Oganian, Y., Schlickeiser, U., Jacobs, A. M., & Heekeren, H. R. (2017). Second language use facilitates implicit emotion regulation via content labeling. Frontiers in psychology, 8, Article 366. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00366
  • Movahedi, S. (1996). Metalinguistic analysis of therapeutic discourse: Flight into a second language when the analyst and the analysand are multilingual. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 44(3), 837-862.
  • Oganian, Y., Korn, C. W., & Heekeren, H. R. (2016). Language switching— but not foreign language use per se—reduces the framing effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 42(1), 140- 148. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000161
  • Pennebaker, J. W., & Chung, C. K. (2011). Expressive writing: Connections to physical and mental health. In H. S. Friedman (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of health psychology (pp. 417–437). Oxford University Press.
  • Phelps, E. A., O’Connor, K. J., Gatenby, J. C., Gore, J. C., Grillon, C., & Davis, M. (2001). Activation of the left amygdala to a cognitive representation of fear. Nature neuroscience, 4(4), 437-441. https://doi.org/10.1038/86110
  • Pitta, P., Marcos, L. R., & Alpert, M. (1978). Language switching as a treatment strategy with bilingual patients. American Journal of Psychoanalysis, 38(3), 255-258 https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01252975
  • R Core Team (2020). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.R-project.org/
  • Rosenblum, S. M. (2011). The role of language in therapy: how bilingual/ multilingual therapists experience their work with bilingual/multilingual clients. [Doctoral thesis. Smith College]. https://scholarworks.smith.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=2090& context=theses
  • Schrauf, R. W. (2000). Bilingual autobiographical memory: Experimental studies and clinical cases. Culture & Psychology, 6(4), 387-417. https://doi.org/10.1177/1354067X0064001
  • Schwanberg, J. S. (2010). Does language of retrieval affect the remembering of trauma?. Journal of Trauma & Dissociation, 11(1), 44-56. https://doi.org/10.1080/15299730903143550
  • Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R., & Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Consulting Psychologists Press.
  • Tabibnia, G., Lieberman, M. D., & Craske, M. G. (2008). The lasting effect of words on feelings: words may facilitate exposure effects to threatening images. Emotion, 8(3), 307. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.8.3.307