Does bold emphasis facilitate the process of visual-word recognition?
- Macaya, María
- Perea Lara, Manuel
ISSN: 1138-7416
Any de publicació: 2014
Volum: 17
Pàgines: 1-5
Tipus: Article
Altres publicacions en: The Spanish Journal of Psychology
Resum
The study of the effects of typographical factors on lexical access has been rather neglected in the literature on visual-word recognition. Indeed, current computational models of visual-word recognition employ an unrefined letter feature level in their coding schemes. In a letter recognition experiment, Pelli, Burns, Farell, and Moore-Page (2006), letters in Bookman boldface produced more efficiency (i.e., a higher ratio of thresholds of an ideal observer versus a human observer) than the letters in Bookman regular under visual noise. Here we examined whether the effect of bold emphasis can be generalized to a common visual-word recognition task (lexical decision: �is the item a word?�) under standard viewing conditions. Each stimulus was presented either with or without bold emphasis (e.g., actor vs. actor). To help determine the locus of the effect of bold emphasis, word-frequency (low vs. high) was also manipulated. Results revealed that responses to words in boldface were faster than the responses to the words without emphasis �this advantage was restricted to low-frequency words. Thus, typographical features play a non-negligible role during visual-word recognition and, hence, the letter feature level of current models of visual-word recognition should be amended.
Referències bibliogràfiques
- Balota D. A., Yap M. J., Cortese M. J., Hutchison K. A., Kessler B., Loftis B.,, Treiman R. (2007). The English lexicon project. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 445-459. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03193014
- Balota D. A., Yap M. J., & Cortese M. J. (2006). Visual word recognition: The journey from features to meaning (A travel update). In M. Traxler & M. A. Gernsbacher (Eds.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (2nd Ed., pp. 285-375). New York, NY: Academic Press.
- Blais C., Fiset D., Jol icoeur P., Arguin M., Bub D. N., & Gosselin F. (2009). Reading between eye saccades. PLoS ONE 4(7): e6448. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal. pone.0006448
- Coltheart M., Rastle K., Perry C., Ziegler J., & Langdon R. (2001). DRC: A dual-route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108, 204-256. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.108.1.204
- Davis C. J., & Perea M. (2005). BuscaPalabras: A program for deriving orthographic and phonological neighborhood statistics and other psycholinguistic indices in Spanish. Behavior Research Methods, 37, 665-671. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03192738
- Davis C. J. (2010). The spatial coding model of visual word identification. Psychological Review, 117, 713-758. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0019738
- Forste r K. I., & Forster J. C. (2003). DMDX: A windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 35, 116-124. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03195503
- Grainger J., & Jacobs A. M. (1996). Orthographic processing in visual word recognition: A multiple read-out model. Psychological Review, 103, 518-565. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.103.3.518
- Hannagan T. , Ktori M., Chanceaux M., & Grainger J. (2012). Deciphering CAPTCHAs: What a turing test reveals about human cognition. Plos One, 7(3), e32121. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0032121
- McClelland J. L., & Rumelhart D. E. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 1. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review, 88, 375-407. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037%2 F0033-295X.88.5.375
- Pelli D. G., Burns C. W., Farell B., & Moore-Page D. C. (2006). Feature detection and letter identification. Vision Research, 46, 4646-4674. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visre s.2006.04.023
- Perea M. (2013). Why does the APA recommend the use of serif fonts? Psicothema, 25, 13-17. http://dx.doi.org/10.7334/psicothema2012.141
- Perea M., & Acha J. (2009). Sp ace information is important for reading. Vision Research, 49, 1994-2000. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2009.05.009
- Perea M., & Gómez P. (2012). Increasing interletter spacing facilitates encoding of words. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 19, 332-338. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13423-011-0214-6
- Perea M., & Rosa E. (2002). Does " whole word shape" play a role in visual word recognition? Perception & Psychophysics, 64, 785-794. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03194745
- Perea M., Comesaña M., & Soares A. P. (2012). Does the advantage of the upper part of words occur at the lexical level Memory and Cognition, 8, 1257-1265. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13421-012-0219-z
- Perea M., Rosa E., & Gómez C. (2005). The frequency effect for pseudowords in the lexical decision task. Perception and Psychophysics, 67, 301-314. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758%2FBF03206493
- Pollatsek A,Well A. D. 1995 On the use of counterbalanced design s in cognitive research: A suggestion for a better and more powerful analysis Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition 21 785-794 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.21.3.785
- Rayner K. (2009). Eye movements and attention in reading, scene perception, and visual search. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 1457-1506. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1747021090281646
- Rumelhart D. E., & Siple P. (1974). The process of recognizing tachistoscopically presented words. Psychological Review, 81, 99-118. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0036117
- Sanocki T., & Dyson M. C. (2012). Letter processing and font information during reading: Beyond distinctiveness, where vision meets design. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74, 132-145. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13414-011-0220-9
- Slattery T. J., & Rayner K. (2010). Eye movements and text legibility. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24, 1129-1148. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp.1623
- Sternberg S. (1969). The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of do nders' method. In W. G. Koster (Ed.), Attention and performance II (pp. 276-315). Amsterdam, The Netherlands. North-Holland Publishing Company.
- Tinker M. A. (1963). Legibility of print. Iowa, IA: Iowa State University Press.
- Van Orden G. C., & Goldinger S. D. (1994). Interdependence of form and function in cognitive systems explains perception of printed words. Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance, 20, 1269-91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523. 20.6.1269