Empatía en la adolescenciaRelaciones con razonamiento moral prosocial, conducta prosocial y agresividad

  1. Tur-Porcar, Ana 1
  2. Llorca, Anna 1
  3. Malonda, Elisabeth 1
  4. Samper, Paula 1
  5. Mestre, María Vicenta 1
  1. 1 Universitat de València
    info

    Universitat de València

    Valencia, España

    ROR https://ror.org/043nxc105

Aldizkaria:
Acción psicológica

ISSN: 1578-908X

Argitalpen urtea: 2016

Zenbakien izenburua: Infancia y adolescencia: Factores de protección y vulnerabilidad [Childhood & Adolescence. Protective & Vulnerability]

Alea: 13

Zenbakia: 2

Orrialdeak: 3-14

Mota: Artikulua

DOI: 10.5944/AP.13.2.17802 DIALNET GOOGLE SCHOLAR lock_openSarbide irekia editor

Beste argitalpen batzuk: Acción psicológica

Garapen Iraunkorreko Helburuak

Laburpena

This study has two objectives. On the one hand, to analyze the relationships between empathy, prosocial behavior and prosocial moral reasoning, as well as the differences between them in terms of gender. On the other hand, to analyze the predictive value of these variables on empathy, taken as a criterion vari­able. The sample comprised 1,557 participants (M = 13.13; DT = .86) from 12 to 15 years (47.4 % girls and 52.6 % boys). Comparison of means shows significant differences based on gender. The girls ex­pressed higher levels of empathy, prosocial behavior, prosocial moral reasoning, in the dimensions of in­ternalized, facing the necessity and stereotyped. The boys, meanwhile, show higher rates in physical and verbal aggressiveness and hedonistic and approval-oriented prosocial moral reasoning. Regression anal­ysis identified as predictors of empathy in boys and girls, prosocial behavior and hedonistic reasoning. Prosocial behavior maintains direct relationships with empathy, in the case of hedonism relationships are negative. In the group of boys there are other predic­tors of empathy. These variables are positively asso­ciated with internalized prosocial moral reasoning and moral reasoning led to the need, and negatively with aggressiveness. Perhaps more importantly, more efficient intervention programs designed to foster specific types of prosocial behaviors.  

Erreferentzia bibliografikoak

  • Bird, C. M., Castelli, F., Malik, O., Frith, U. y Husain, M. (2004). The impact of extensive medical frontal lobe damage on ‘Theory of Mind’ and cognition. Brain, 127(4), 914-928. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awh108
  • Bryant, B. K. (1982). An index of empathy for children and adolescents. Child Development, 53, 413-425. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1128984
  • Bzdok, D., Schilbach, L., Vogeley, K., Schneider, K., Laird, A. R., Langner, R. y Eickhoff, S. B. (2012). Parsing the neural correlates of moral cognition: ALE meta-analysis on morality, theory of mind, and empathy. Brain Structure and Function, 217, 783-796. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00429-0120380-y
  • Caprara, G. V. y Pastorelli, C. (1993). Early emotional instability, prosocial behavior, and aggression: some methodological aspects. European Journal of Personality, 7, 19-36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/per.2410070103
  • Carlo, G. y Randall, B. A. (2002). The Development of a Measure of Prosocial Behaviors for Late Adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 31(1), 31-44. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1014033032440
  • Carlo, G. (2006). Care-based and altruistically-based morality. En M. Killen y J. G. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (pp. 551-579). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Carlo, G., Eisenberg, N. y Knight, G. P. (1992). An objective measure of adolescents prosocial moral reasoning. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 2(4), 331-349.
  • Carlo, G., Hausman, A., Christiansen, S. y Randall, B. (2003). Cognitive and behavioral correlates of a measure of prosocial tendencies for adolescent. Journal of Early Adolescence, 23(1), 107-134.
  • Carlo, G., Mestre, M. V., McGinley, M. M., Tur-Porcar, A., Samper, P. y Opal, D. (2014). The protective role of prosocial behaviors on antisocial behaviors: The mediating effects of deviant peer affiliation. Journal of Adolescence, 37(4), 359-366. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2014.02.009
  • Carlo, G., Mestre, M. V., McGinley, M., Tur-Porcar, A., Samper, P. y Streit, C. (2013). The structure and correlates of a measure of prosocial moral reasoning in adolescents from Spain. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 10(2), 174-189. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2012.762909
  • Carlo, G., Mestre, M. V., Samper, P., Tur, A. y Armenta, B. E. (2010). Feelings or cognitions? Moral cognitions and emotions as longitudinal predictors of prosocial and aggressive behaviors. Personality and Individual Differences, 48(8), 865-962. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.02.010
  • Chakrabarti, B. y Baron-Cohen, S. (2006). Empathizing: Neurocognitive developmental mechanisms and individual differences. Progress in Brain Research, 156, 403-417. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S00796123(06)56022-4
  • Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1), 155-159. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155
  • Cornelio-Nieto, J. O. (2009). Autismo infantil y neuronas en espejo. Revista de Neurología, 48(2), 27-29.
  • Davis, M. H. (1980). A Multidimensional Approach to Individual Differences in Empathy. Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 10(85), 1-17. Recuperado de http://www.uv.es/triasnav/Davis_1980.pdf.
  • De Wied, M., Branje, S. y Meeus, W. (2007). Empathy and conflict resolution in friendship relations among adolescents. Aggressive Behavior, 33, 4855. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ab.20166
  • Del Barrio, M. V., Moreno, C. y López, R. (2001). Evaluación de la agresión e inestabilidad emocional en niños españoles y su relación con la depresión. Clínica y Salud, 12(1), 33-50.
  • Eagly, A. H. (2009). The his and hers of prosocial behavior: An examination of the social psychology of gender. American Psychologist, 64, 644–658. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066x.64.8.644
  • Eisenberg, N. y Strayer, J. (1987). Empathy and its development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Eisenberg, N. (1986). Altruistic emotion, cognition and behavior. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Eisenberg, N., Carlo, G., Murphy, B. y Van Court, P. (1995). Prosocial development in late adolescence: A longitudinal Study. Child Development, 66(4), 1179-1197. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.2307/1131806
  • Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., Guthrie, I. K., Murphy, B. C. y Shepard, S. A. (2005). Age changes in prosocial responding and moral reasoning in adolescence and early adulthood. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 15(3), 235-260. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7795.2005.00095.x
  • Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A. y Spinrad, T. L. (2006). Prosocial development. En N. Eisenberg, W. Damon y R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology. Vol. 3: Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., pp. 646-718). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
  • Eisenberg, N., Guthrie, I. K., Cumberland, A., Murphy, B. C., Shepard, S. A., Zhou, Q. y Carlo, G. (2002). Prosocial Development in Early Adulthood: A longitudinal Study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(6), 993-1006. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.82.6.993
  • Eisenberg, N., Hofer, C., Sulik, M. J. y Liew, J. (2014). The development of prosocial moral reasoning and a prosocial orientation in young adulthood: Concurrent and longitudinal correlates. Developmental Psychology, 50(1), 58-70. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0032990
  • Eisenberg, N., Zhou, Q. y Koller, S. (2001). Brazilian adolescents’ prosocial moral judgment and behaviour: Relations to sympathy, perspective taking, gender-role orientation, and demographic characteristics. Child Development, 72(2), 518-534. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00294
  • Fernández-Pinto, I., López-Pérez, B. y Márquez, M. (2008). Empatía: Medidas, teorías y aplicaciones en revisión. Anales de Psicología, 24(2), 284-298.
  • Garaigordobil, M. y García de Galdeano, P. (2006). Empatía en niños de 10 a 12 años. Psicothema, 18(2), 180-186.
  • Gaylord, N. K., Kitzmann, M. y Coleman, J. K. (2003). Parents’ and children’s perceptions of parental behavior: associations with children’s psychological adjustment in the classroom. Parenting: Science and Practice, 3(1), 23-47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15327922PAR0301_02
  • Gilligan, C. (1982). In a different voice: psychological theory and women’s development. Cambridge, MA, Harvard University Press.
  • Gutiérrez, M., Escartí, A. y Pascual, C. (2011). Relaciones entre empatía, conducta prosocial, agresividad, autoeficacia y responsabilidad personal y social de los escolares. Psicothema, 23(1), 13-19.
  • Hare, R. D. (2006). Psychopathy: A clinical and forensic overview. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 29(3), 709-724. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psc.2006.04.007
  • Igartua, J. J. y Páez, R. D. (1998). Validez y fiabilidad de una escala de empatía e identificación con los personajes. Psicothema, 10(2), 423-436.
  • Jabbi, M., Swart, M. y Keysers, C. (2007). Empathy for positive and negative emotions in the gustatory cortex. Neuroimagen, 34(4), 1744-1753. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.10.032
  • Klimecki, O. M., Leiberg, S., Ricard, M. y Singer, T. (2014). Differential pattern of functional brain plasticity after compassion and empathy training. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(6), 873-879. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/scan/nst060
  • Knafo, A. y Plomin, R. (2006). Prosocial behavior from early to middle childhood: Genetic and environmental influences on stability and change. Developmental Psychology, 42, 771-786. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.42.5.771
  • Litvack, M. W., McDougall, D. y Romney, D. M. (1997). The structure of empathy during middle childhood and its relationship to prosocial behavior. Genetic, Social and General Psychopatology Monographs, 123(3), 303-324.
  • Malti, T. y Keller, M. (2009). The relation of elementary school children’s externalizing behaviour to emotion attributions, evaluation of consequences, and moral reasoning. European Journal of Developmental Science, 6, 592-614. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17405620701497497
  • Mehrabian, A. y Epstein, N. (1972). A measure of emotional empathy. Journal of Personality, 40(4), 525-543. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.14676494.1972.tb00078.x
  • Mestre, M. V., Frías, D., Samper, P. y Tur, A. M. (2002). Adaptación y validación en población española del PROM: una medida objetiva del razonamiento moral prosocial. Acción Psicológica, 1(3), 221-232. http://dx.doi.org/10.5944/ap.1.3.554
  • Mestre, M. V., Frías, D., Samper, P. y Tur, A. M. (2004). Las medidas de la empatía, análisis del Interpersonal Reactivity Index. Psicothema, 16(2), 255-260.
  • Mestre, M. V., Tur, A., Samper, P. y Malonda, E. (2011). Programa de educación de las emociones: la convivencia. Valencia, España: Tirant lo Blanch.
  • Mestre, M. V., Tur, A., Samper, P., Nácher, M. J. y Cortés, M. T. (2007). Estilos de crianza en la adolescencia y su relación con el comportamiento prosocial. Revista Latinoamericana de Psicología, 39(2), 211-225. http://dx.doi.org/10.14349/rlp.v39i2.434
  • Mestre, V., Pérez, E., Frías, D. y Samper, P. (1999). Instrumentos de evaluación de la empatía. En E. Pérez Delgado y V. Mestre, Psicología moral y crecimiento personal (pp. 181-190). Barcelona, España: Ariel.
  • Mestre, V., Samper, P., Frías, D. y Tur, A. (2009). Are women more empathetic than men? A longitudinal study in adolescence. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 12(1), 76-83.
  • Mikolajewski, A. J., Chavarria, J., Moltisanti, A., Hart, S. A. y Taylor, J. (2014). Examining the factor structure and etiology of prosociality. Psychological Assessment, 26(4), 1259-1267. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0037132
  • Oberman, L. M., Ramachandran, V. S. y Pineda, J. A. (2008). Modulation of mu suppression in children with autism spectrum disorders in response to familiar or unfamiliar stimuli: The mirror neuron hypothesis. Neuropsychologia 46(5), 1558-1565. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.0 1.010
  • Pardo, A. y Ruiz, M. A. (2005). Análisis de datos con SPSS 15. Base. Madrid, España: McGraw-Hill.
  • Romersi, S., Martínez-Fernández, J. R. y Roche, R. (2011). Efectos del Programa Mínimo de Incremento Prosocial en una muestra de estudiantes de educación secundaria. Anales de Psicología, 27(1), 135-146.
  • Silfver, M., Helkama, K., Lönnqvist, J. y Verkasalo, M. (2008). The relation between value priorities and proneness to guilt, shame and empathy. Motivation and Emotion, 32, 69-80. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11031-008-9084-2
  • Strayer, J. y Roberts, W. (2004). Empathy and observed anger and aggression in five-year-olds. Social Development, 13(1), 1-13. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2004.00254.x
  • Telle, N. T. y Pfister, H. R. (2016). Positive Empathy and Prosocial Behavior: A Neglected Link. Emotion Review, 8(2), 154-163. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1754073915586817
  • Tur, A. M. (2003). Conducta agresiva y prosocial en relación con temperamento y hábitos de crianza en niños y adolescentes (Tesis Doctoral). Universidad de Valencia, España. Recuperado de http://roderic.uv.es/handle/10550/38891.